Por favor, use este identificador para citar o enlazar este ítem: https://hdl.handle.net/10495/42585
Registro completo de metadatos
Campo DC Valor Lengua/Idioma
dc.contributor.authorTrujillo Orrego, Natalia-
dc.contributor.authorGómez Londoño, Diana-
dc.contributor.authorTrujillo Orrego, Sandra Patricia-
dc.contributor.authorIbañez Barassi, Agustín-
dc.contributor.authorLópez Hincapié, José David-
dc.contributor.authorParra Rodríguez, Mario Alfredo-
dc.date.accessioned2024-10-02T18:28:05Z-
dc.date.available2024-10-02T18:28:05Z-
dc.date.issued2021-
dc.identifier.citationTrujillo N, Gómez D, Trujillo S, López JD, Ibáñez A, Parra MA. Attentional bias during emotional processing: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence from an Emotional Flanker Task. PLoS One. 2021 Apr 2;16(4):e0249407. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0249407.spa
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/10495/42585-
dc.description.abstractABSTRACT: Threatening stimuli seem to capture attention more swiftly than neutral stimuli. This attention bias has been observed under different experimental conditions and with different types of stimuli. It remains unclear whether this adaptive behaviour reflects the function of automatic or controlled attention mechanisms. Additionally, the spatiotemporal dynamics of its neural correlates are largely unknown. The present study investigates these issues using an Emotional Flanker Task synchronized with EEG recordings. A group of 32 healthy participants saw response-relevant images (emotional scenes from IAPS or line drawings of objects) flanked by response-irrelevant distractors (i.e., emotional scenes flanked by line drawings or vice versa). We assessed behavioural and ERP responses drawn from four task conditions (Threat-Central, Neutral-Central, Threat-Peripheral, and Neutral-Peripheral) and subjected these responses to repeated-measures ANOVA models. When presented as response-relevant targets, threatening images attracted faster and more accurate responses. They did not affect response accuracy to targets when presented as response irrelevant flankers. However, response times were significantly slower when threatening images flanked objects than when neutral images were shown as flankers. This result replicated the well-known Emotional Flanker Effect. Behavioural responses to response-relevant threatening targets were accompanied by significant modulations of ERP activity across all time-windows and regions of interest and displayed some meaningful correlations. The Emotional Flanker Effect was accompanied by a modulation over parietal and central-parietal regions within a time-window between 550-690 ms. Such a modulation suggests that the attentional disruption to targets caused by response-irrelevant threatening flankers appears to reflect less neural resources available, which are seemingly drawn away by distracting threatening flankers. The observed spatiotemporal dynamics seem to concur with understanding of the important adaptive role attributed to threat-related attention bias.spa
dc.format.extent20 páginasspa
dc.format.mimetypeapplication/pdfspa
dc.language.isoengspa
dc.publisherPublic Library of Sciencespa
dc.type.hasversioninfo:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersionspa
dc.rightsinfo:eu-repo/semantics/openAccessspa
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.5/co/*
dc.titleAttentional bias during emotional processing: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence from an Emotional Flanker Taskspa
dc.typeinfo:eu-repo/semantics/articlespa
dc.publisher.groupGrupo de Neurociencias de Antioquiaspa
dc.publisher.groupSalud Mentalspa
dc.publisher.groupSistemas Embebidos e Inteligencia Computacional (SISTEMIC)spa
dc.identifier.doi10.1371/journal.pone.0249407-
oaire.versionhttp://purl.org/coar/version/c_970fb48d4fbd8a85spa
dc.rights.accessrightshttp://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2spa
dc.identifier.eissn1932-6203-
oaire.citationtitlePLoS ONEspa
oaire.citationstartpage1spa
oaire.citationendpage20spa
oaire.citationvolume16spa
oaire.citationissue4spa
dc.rights.creativecommonshttps://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/spa
oaire.fundernameColombia. Ministerio de Ciencia, Tecnología e Innovación - MinCienciasspa
oaire.fundernameNational Institutes of Healthspa
dc.publisher.placeSan Francisco, Esatdos Unidosspa
dc.type.coarhttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_2df8fbb1spa
dc.type.redcolhttps://purl.org/redcol/resource_type/ARTspa
dc.type.localArtículo de investigaciónspa
dc.subject.decsAtención - fisiología-
dc.subject.decsAttention - physiology-
dc.subject.decsSesgo Atencional-
dc.subject.decsAttentional Bias-
dc.subject.decsElectroencefalografía-
dc.subject.decsElectroencephalography-
dc.subject.decsEmociones - fisiología-
dc.subject.decsEmotions - physiology-
dc.subject.decsPotenciales Evocados-
dc.subject.decsEvoked Potentials-
dc.subject.decsEstimulación Luminosa-
dc.subject.decsPhotic Stimulation-
dc.subject.decsTiempo de Reacción-
dc.subject.decsReaction Time-
dc.description.researchgroupidCOL0015983spa
dc.description.researchgroupidCOL0010717spa
dc.description.researchgroupidCOL0010744spa
oaire.awardnumberMinCiencias 111577757638spa
oaire.awardnumberNIH R01 AG057234spa
dc.subject.meshurihttps://id.nlm.nih.gov/mesh/D001288-
dc.subject.meshurihttps://id.nlm.nih.gov/mesh/D000070379-
dc.subject.meshurihttps://id.nlm.nih.gov/mesh/D004569-
dc.subject.meshurihttps://id.nlm.nih.gov/mesh/D004644-
dc.subject.meshurihttps://id.nlm.nih.gov/mesh/D005071-
dc.subject.meshurihttps://id.nlm.nih.gov/mesh/D010775-
dc.subject.meshurihttps://id.nlm.nih.gov/mesh/D011930-
dc.relation.ispartofjournalabbrevPLoS ONEspa
oaire.funderidentifier.rorRoR:03fd5ne08-
oaire.funderidentifier.rorRoR:01cwqze88-
Aparece en las colecciones: Artículos de Revista en Salud Pública

Ficheros en este ítem:
Fichero Descripción Tamaño Formato  
TrujilloNatalia_2021_Attentional_Bias_Emotional.pdfArtículo de investigación1.54 MBAdobe PDFVisualizar/Abrir


Este ítem está sujeto a una licencia Creative Commons Licencia Creative Commons Creative Commons